http://www.cultsock.ndirect.co.uk/MUHome/cshtml/media/kl.html
|
Mass
media: limited effects |
Katz and Lazarsfeld: Two-Step Flow
The study of the 1940 election campaign
In 1940, Lazarsfeld,
Berelson and Gaudet conducted the first full-scale investigation of the
effects of political mass communication. Their research focused on the
1940 Presidential election campaign and their findings were published in
1944 in The People's Choice after more research had been conducted.
The importance of social influence
Their research was originally based on something like the simplistic hypodermic
needle model of media influence, whereby it was assumed that a message
would be transmitted from the mass media to a 'mass audience', who would
absorb the message. However, their investigations suggested that media
effects were minimal, that the conception of a 'mass audience' was inadequate
and misguided and that social influences had a major effect on the process
of opinion formation and sharply limited the media's effect.
Limited effects
The study by Lazarsfeld et al concluded that only some 5% of people changed
their voting behaviour as a result of media messages. Their exposure to
election broadcasts turned out to be a relatively poor predictor of their
voting behaviour, particularly when compared with other factors such as
their interpersonal communication with friends, union members, business
colleagues and the political tradition they had grown up in. This view
of media effects was confirmed n a variety of other investigations and
came to be known as the 'limited effects paradigm' of media influence.
Two-Step Flow: general conclusions
Consequently Lazarsfeld and his colleagues developed the notion of a 'two-step'
flow of media messages, a process in which opinion leaders played a vitally
important rôle.
This was later developed by Katz and Lazarsfeld and presented in their
book Personal Influence (1955). A number of significant conclusions
follow from their research:
-
our responses to media messages will be mediated through our social
relationships, the effects of media messages being sharply limited
by interpersonal relationships and group membership (this is confirmed
also by Hovland
who
identifies our adherence to group norms as a major factor; see also the
more general sections on Social
Influence)
-
it is misleading to think of receivers as members of a 'mass audience'
since that implies that they are all equal in their reception of media
messages, whereas in fact some play a more active rôle than others
-
receiving a message does not imply responding to it; nor does non-reception
imply non-response (since we may still receive the message via interpersonal
communication)
-
there are some people amongst the media audience who act as opinion
leaders - typically such people use the mass media more than the average,
mix more than the average across social classes and see themselves and
are seen by others as having an influence on others
Reasons suggested for the greater effectiveness of personal influence over
media influence include the following:
-
The content and development of a conversation are less predictable than
mass media messages. Consequently, the receiver cannot be as selective
in advance as (s)he is able to be when choosing which media messages to
attend to.
-
In a face-to-face conversation, the critical distance between the partners
is less than in mass communication.
-
By direct questioning of the partner in the conversation, the assumptions
underlying the conversation can be rapidly and accurately established,
which is not so with mass communication.
-
In face-to-face interaction the communicator can rapidly adjust to the
receiver's personality. (S)he has direct feedback as to the success of
the communication, can correct misunderstandings and counter challenges.
Criticisms
The
model is often presented graphically as shown on the right. In fact, that
is somewhat misleading as it suggests that mass media messages flow first
to opinion leaders and from them to the rest. Obviously, that's not the
case, since you and I can both receive messages directly. The point is
that the messages we receive are then modified through the pattern of our
social contacts.
Katz and Lazarsfeld are perhaps also somewhat misleading when they suggest
that individuals with certain characteristics are opinion leaders. It may
be the case that many opinion leaders will have the characteristics they
mention, but we also know that some opinion leaders in some subject areas
will not have those general characteristics. However, I should mention
that Katz and Lazarsfeld certainly did not take the view that opinion leaders
were necessarily those formally recognized as such (e. g. celebrities,
politicians etc.) Thus, their studies showed that top-down influence was
relatively slight. Influence tended to be horizontal across a particular
socio-economic class, except that in the 'higher' social classes there
was a tendency for people to find opinion leaders in the next class up.
No opinion leader was an opinion leader in all aspects of life. For example,
the car mechanic in your local pub may not use the media much at all because
he's always working late. Nevertheless, he knows a lot about cars and so
what the rest of those in the pub 'know' from the media about different
makes of car will be influenced by his views. Similarly, your Politics
lecturer may not use the media anything like as much as you do, but her
reading and viewing is targeted on political issues. Together with her
broad knowledge of political theory and history, that is likely to make
her an opinion leader as far as your Politics class is concerned. Allowing
for those differences from one class to another and from one subject area
to another, we probably can recognize in opinion leaders the characterisitics
which Katz and Lazarsfeld suggested, in particular that opinion leaders
will be more active users of the mass media than others.
Katz and Lazarsfeld may also be misleading in suggesting that people
are either active opinion leaders or passive followers of opinion leaders.
Apart from the evidence that people can be opinion leaders on some matters
and not on others, there is also the objection that some people may be
neither leaders nor followers, but quite simply detached from much media
output.
Katz and Lazarsfeld's Influence
Despite those and other criticisms, the fact remains that Katz and Lazarsfeld's
research is widely accepted and still highly influential. Advertisers and
spin doctors recognise that 'the best form of advertising is word-of-mouth
advertising'. So, for example, in the 1992 General Election campaign both
parties were keen to present 'facts' and 'true stories' in their TV campaign
materials. Their aim was not really to persuade their opponents (they would
very probably not watch the broadcasts anyway), but rather to provide the
opinion leaders amongst their supporters with ammunition in support of
their arguments down the pub, at work and so on. Similarly, in preparation
for the launch of the 'Poll Tax', the Conservative Party was keen to make
contact with local councillors and other local opinion leaders in addition
to the conventional media campaign. In the 1997 British election campaign
it was apparent that the two main British parties were concerned to address
their arguments to the small number of voters in winnable constituencies.
Thus the election campaign was in effect addressed to around a quarter
of a million voters only. Face-to-face communication is ideally also organised
to support the mass media message - for example, in third world education
programmes, people are brought together to watch a film, listen to a radio
broadcast etc.
Introduction to Mass Media Effects
Glossary of media studies terms